In most studies, a particular stimulus feature is always associat

In most studies, a particular stimulus feature is always associated with a particular response and optimum performance is signified by the maximum possible d′ value GSK 3 inhibitor (typically between 3 and 4). Because of the family resemblance structure employed here, each feature was only associated with its typical category on 78% of trials. As a consequence, the optimum d′ score was lower: a participant classifying with 100% accuracy would have d′ scores of 1.52 for each dimension (indicated by the blue line in Fig. 4A). Scores higher than this indicate an over-extension of the learning in the strongest dimension, such that the information in this dimension was driving classification even for exemplars

where the other two dimensions pointed towards a different category. This over-generalisation was present in four of the seven patients and is similar to the over-generalisation exhibited by SD patients when attempting to use their impaired conceptual knowledge of real objects (see Discussion). No patients demonstrated much learning Small molecule library in their second or weakest dimensions, in line with the prediction that they would be unable to form category representations that integrated all of the information required for optimum categorisation. The mean d′ scores in each group can be seen in Fig. 4A. As expected, there was a

large disparity between the strongest dimension and the remaining two dimensions in SD, with a more balanced pattern of learning across the three dimensions in the control group. A 3 (dimension) × 2 (group) ANOVA was performed on these data. There was a main effect of dimension [F(2,34) = 43, p < .001]. There was no effect of group but there was a highly significant interaction between dimension and group [F(2,34) = 6.83, p = .003]. Post-hoc t-tests indicated that SD patients showed significantly less learning on their weakest dimension than controls [t(17) = 3.44, p = .003]. There was also a trend towards poorer learning on the second dimension in SD patients, relative to controls

[t(17) = 1.95, p = .07]. While the general pattern in the patient group was towards strong, single-dimension learning, we did observe some variation across patients, with J.W., N.H. and E.T. displaying a less clear pattern than the other four ADAMTS5 patients. To investigate these differences, we tested whether these patients’ responses were influenced by the shape colour dimension, which was irrelevant for classification. We calculated a d′ measure of “learning” in this dimension in a similar manner to the other dimensions. Since this dimension was irrelevant to classification, the optimum d′ was 0. The results are shown in Fig. 4B. The four patients who achieved the most successful learning on their strongest dimension showed low d′ values, indicating that they were not influenced by the irrelevant dimension. However, patients N.H. and E.T., and to a lesser extent J.W.

To detect the differences in macrophage differentiation,

To detect the differences in macrophage differentiation,

ANOVA for paired samples was used, followed by Fisher’s least significant different test. Correlations were Protease Inhibitor Library clinical trial evaluated by Spearman’s test. The criterion of significance was set to p < 0.05. To investigate the effect of soluble Aβ-peptides on the phagocytosis of PSPs by freshly isolated human monocytes, the cells were pre-incubated with 1 μg/ml of the respective Aβ-peptide in cell culture medium. Then, 20 h after adding the fluorescent PSPs, phagocytosis was quantified by flow cytometry. The MFI of the phagocytes was used as a measure of the number of internalized fluorescent particles. The pre-incubation of monocytes with Aβ(1–42) as well as the N-terminally truncated Aβ(2–40) and Aβ(2–42), but not Aβ(1–40), induced phagocytosis at levels significantly above the control levels (p < 0.05) ( Fig. 1A). Monocytes treated with Aβ(2–40) internalized 17% more PSPs than those treated with full-length Aβ(1–40) (p < 0.05). The treatment of cells

with BSA did not influence the phagocytosis of PSPs. To assess whether the Aβ-peptides secreted AZD6244 cell line by monocytes enhanced phagocytosis by binding to pathogens, the effect of Aβ-coated PSPs on their phagocytosis by human monocytes was examined. The phagocytosis of fluorescent particles was quantified by flow cytometry as described above (Fig. 1B). Precoating the fluorescent PSPs with all of the tested Aβ-peptides increased their phagocytosis by monocytes compared to the phagocytosis of uncoated PSP (p < 0.001). Coating the PSPs with Aβ(1–42) enhanced the amount of phagocytosed PSPs by 40% (p < 0.0001). Aβ(1–42) induced phagocytosis more effectively than Aβ(1–40) (p < 0.0001). The treatment

of monocytes with Aβ(2–42)− and Aβ(3p–42)-coated PSPs resulted in an even higher increase of the MFI values by 53% (p < 0.0001) and 56% (p < 0.0001), respectively. This result indicates that an additional aminophylline N-truncation of Aβ(1–42) further increased the phagocytosis of PSPs. In contrast to the treatment of monocytes with soluble Aβ-peptides, pre-incubation with n-truncated Aβ(2–40) did not enhance phagocytosis more effectively than Aβ(1–40). Because undifferentiated monocytes are poor phagocytes, cytochalasin D only weakly reduced phagocytosis. Phagocytosis of pHrodo Green-labeled E. coli, which is only fluorescent at an acidic pH, revealed that cytochalasin D completely inhibited phagocytosis in our setting ( Fig. 4F). Therefore, increased signal intensity after pretreatment with cytochalasin D and coincubation with permanently fluorescent prey indicated its binding to the phagocyte surface without internalization. To investigate whether the differential effects of the Aβ-peptides were due to different binding affinities for the PSPs, the amount of Aβ-peptide bound to the PSPs was quantified by immunostaining with the C- and N-terminal non-specific Aβ antibodies 6E10 and 4G8.

While catch levels overall can appear relatively stable, a number

While catch levels overall can appear relatively stable, a number of species have undergone such regional declines that their fisheries have collapsed. Alfonsino fisheries Selleck PF-562271 off the Azores and Corner Rise seamounts in the 1970s by the former Soviet Union lasted only a few years, and a spawning location for blue ling in the North Atlantic yielded 8000 t in one year before ceasing as catches dropped rapidly [80]. In the western North Atlantic, the three species of wolffish, and cusk, have reported declines in stock size of over 90% within the time period of three generations, and 38% of deep-sea bottom fish species in that area could be “at-risk”

based on the extent of population declines in surveys [29]. Yet off New Zealand, oreo fisheries have had relatively stable landings for an extended period, and current stock MAPK inhibitor status for both major commercial species

is estimated to be around 50% of unfished levels [36]. Hence, fisheries can be sustained where life history characteristics are known and appropriate management is applied to limit catches and/or effort levels. Precious corals are caught in some deep-sea fishing operations. They have been sought for use as adornments for millennia in Mediterranean countries. Today, black, pink/red and gold corals (Antipathidae, Corallidae and Zoanthidae) are collected for the jewelry trade in the Mediterranean, India, Japan, Pacific Islands, Hawaii and the Caribbean. In the Pacific Island region, collecting is generally done selectively using scuba or submersibles, and the precious coral “beds” are protected from overfishing [105] and [106], though lack of profitability has halted this Liothyronine Sodium fishery in recent years. Deep-sea corals are also landed in large quantities

as unwanted bycatch in other fisheries [107], [108] and [109]. For example, between 1990 and 2002, Alaskan fisheries, primarily in the Aleutian Islands, landed approximately 4186 t of corals and sponges, with ∼90% removed by bottom trawling [110]. In British Columbia, between 1996 and 2002, at least 15 hauls took over 4 t apiece. Orange roughy trawling on the South Tasman Rise seamounts (adjacent to the Australia EEZ) landed 1.6 t of coral per hour during the first year of the fishery (1997–1998). Indeed, in the first year they took over 1100 t of corals, a coral bycatch about 25% of the orange roughy catch [107]. Coral bycatch is highest when trawling moves into a previously unfished area, then rapidly declines. From a conservation perspective, therefore, reduced coral bycatch is not necessarily a good sign. Although short-term effects of bottom trawling are now widely known [111], [112] and [113], there have been limited studies on long-term impacts [114]. Estimated recovery rates depend on the life history of a particular organism, and range from one to five times their generation time [115].

Interestingly, in the evolved KE07 variant some mutated residues

Interestingly, in the evolved KE07 variant some mutated residues destabilized the transition state (Figure 2). Residue contributions to the reorganization energy were used to screen for mutations that facilitate evolution of the

original KE07 design. Residues, which did not compromise the reorganization energy were selected [28•]. The predicted mutations were in agreement with libraries of active variants from different rounds of directed evolution [37]. This indicates that screening should also allow those residues, which are not involved in catalysis directly, but enable structural changes required along the pathway. KE07 analysis also demonstrates that reorganization energy can be optimized during evolution via small rotamer changes and smaller scale rearrangements in the electrostatic interaction pattern. Besides KE07 Kemp eliminase, further examples selective HDAC inhibitors indicate that electrostatic preorganization could be tuned in directed evolution [6••, 31 and 32••]. This implies that variants, where the preorganization

effect was maximized, could serve as promising starting points for further laboratory optimization. As reorganization energy is invested upon protein folding [44], so evaluating it could affect scaffold ranking and selection. The proposed flowchart of the computer aided design complemented by reorganization energy calculations is shown in Figure 3. First, ab initio calculations are employed to determine the reaction mechanism, the TS geometry and the parameters for the energy functionals for the reactant and the product state. Second, a high-throughput scaffold search is performed based on shape complementarity this website from and TS binding energy. Third, global reorganization energy is computed for top-ranked scaffolds, and will serve as a basis of filtering. Selected variants will be further optimized based on comparing individual reorganization energy contributions of the original and mutated residues. Successful enzyme designs provide insights

into how catalysis can be evoked. The performance of artificial enzymes varies in a wide range, but even with the assistance of directed evolution remains inferior to natural enzymes. Moderate efficiency of man-made constructs indicates the absence of a major catalytic factor, which can also be optimized in laboratory. Electrostatic preorganization has dominant contribution to the catalytic effect and it can also be significantly improved by directed evolution. On the basis of the reactant and product energy functions, reorganization energy can be computed in an economical manner and individual contributions can be determined. We propose to utilize global reorganization energy for refinement and final evaluation of top-ranked scaffolds. Screening based on individual contributions can result in variants similar to evolved libraries, which also include stabilizing or compensatory mutations in addition to those, which have direct impact on catalysis.

Microparticles of plastics

are derived from this brittle

Microparticles of plastics

are derived from this brittle surface layer. Surface microcracking is commonly observed in UV-exposed plastics including HDPE (Akay et al., 1980), LDPE www.selleckchem.com/products/Adrucil(Fluorouracil).html (Küpper et al., 2004 and Tavares et al., 2003), polycarbonate (Blaga and Yamasaki, 1976) and polypropylene (Qayyum and White, 1993 and Yakimets et al., 2004). Consistent with these findings, extensive microcracking and pitting is reported on mesoplastic debris collected from beaches as well (Cooper and Corcoran, 2010, Gregory, 1983 and Ogata et al., 2009). Polypropylene rope sample that had weathered on a pier for several years (provided courtesy of Capt. Charles Moore, Algalita Marine Foundation) when extracted with distiled water yielded large amounts of plastic microplastics that were visualised by staining with Nile Red (Andrady, 2010). The same degradation does not occur in plastics exposed while floating in water. As pointed out already, the low water temperature and foulant effects retard the process dramatically. Plastics that are directly www.selleckchem.com/products/azd5363.html discarded into the water (from vessels) or litter washed into the water prior to any significant weathering degradation are also unlikely to yield microplastics via this mechanism. The same is true of plastics debris that sink in the water

column. The lack of UV-B (rapidly attenuated in sea water) to initiate the process, the low temperatures and the lower oxygen concentration relative to that in air, makes extensive degradation far less likely than for the floating plastics debris. Thus the most likely site for generation of microplastics in the marine environment is the beach. Recognition that microparticles (and

therefore also nanoplastics) are most likely generated on beaches underlines the importance of beach cleaning as an effective mitigation strategy. The removal of larger pieces of plastic debris from beaches before these are weathered enough to be surface embrittled can have considerable value in reducing the microplastics that end up in the ocean. Beach cleanup therefore can have an ecological benefit far beyond the aesthetic improvements of the beaches, and by reducing microplastics, contributes towards the health of the marine food web. Sea water already contains numerous natural buy Bortezomib micro- and nanoparticles (∼106–107 particles per ml or 10–500 μg/l) most of them <100 nm in size (Rosse and Loizeau, 2003). Filter feeders in the ocean ranging from the nano-zooplanktons to Balleen Whales, routinely interact with these without any apparent ill effect. As no enzymatic pathways available to break down the synthetic polymers in any of these organisms, ingested of microplastics are also never digested or absorbed and should therefore be bio-inert. Ingestion of microplastics by microbiota, however, presents a very different problem.

, 2013 and Chitsaz et al , 2011) it is likely that these taxa als

, 2013 and Chitsaz et al., 2011) it is likely that these taxa also comprise ecologically distinct lineages. Conversely, the recently characterized SAR11 1C, or deep SAR11 clade, maintains high conservation of gene content and gene order when compared

to surface clades (Thrash et al., 2014) indicating that it employs a similar metabolic strategy. The majority of the organic carbon remineralization occurs below the photic zone (del Giorgio and Duarte, 2002) and genes associated with a particle attached lifestyle such as pilus synthesis, protein export, and polysaccharide and antibiotic synthesis genes, appear to be relatively more abundant in deep than surface waters (DeLong et al., 2006). There is also considerable autotrophic carbon assimilation or primary production

in the deep ocean (e.g. Karl et al., 1984, Walsh et al., 2009, Swan et al., 2011 and Anantharaman et al., Selleck BIBW2992 2013). This capacity is apparent selleck screening library in many common and abundant deep sea lineages including the deltaproteobacterial SAR324 clade, and the gammaproteobacterial ARCTIC96BD-19, SUP05, Agg54 and Oceanospirillum clades ( Walsh et al., 2009, Swan et al., 2011 and Anantharaman et al., 2013). These organisms possess genes consistent with the ability to utilize dissimilatory sulfur oxidation for energetic support of autotrophic carbon fixation ( Walsh et al., 2009 and Swan et al., 2011). Mixotrophy and metabolic flexibility appear to be common lifestyle traits

enabling successful habitation of the deep sea. All the above organisms are capable of heterotrophy and, at least for the SAR324, sulfur oxidation and carbon fixation as well as C1 utilization and heterotrophy may all operate in a population simultaneously ADP ribosylation factor ( Sheik et al., 2014). Similarly, the highly abundant heterotrophic Thaumarchaeota also display significant chemoautotrophic metabolism, fuelled by oxidation of ammonia to nitrite ( Berg et al., 2007). Genomic plasticity in the SUP05 clade enables this group to optimize its energy metabolism to suite its local environment. For example, genes involved for H2 and sulfur oxidation are over expressed in hydrothermal plumes, an environment where these electron donors are enriched, while in the background deep-sea a second hydrogenase is more prevalent ( Anantharaman et al., 2013). While many traits have distributions that correlate strictly with the taxonomic structure of the underlying community, such as the variations in photosynthetic capacity described within the picocyanobacteria, other traits, such as nitrogen fixation (e.g. Mahaffey et al., 2005), display a habitat-dependant but taxon-independent distribution. Indeed, several re-analyses of the GOS metagenomics datasets examining different levels of metabolic complexity, including pathways, modules and operons (Gianoulis et al.

The ANOVA on the data from the 1000–2000 msec interval gave rise

The ANOVA on the data from the 1000–2000 msec interval gave rise to a significant interaction between discrimination difficulty, subsequent

memory and scalp location [F(1, 27) = 6.82, p = .015], which was further modulated by electrode site [F(5.2, 140.4) = 3.03, p = .011]. Separate analyses in each discrimination difficulty condition revealed an interaction between subsequent memory and scalp location for the easy condition [F(1, 27) = 11.73, p = .002]. This interaction reflected a negative-going subsequent memory effect at anterior [F(1, 27) = 5.32, p = .029] but not posterior (p = .482) locations. Visual and auditory cues involving a difficult discrimination www.selleckchem.com/products/SP600125.html did again not elicit significant encoding-related effects (p > .216). No significant effects emerged in proximity of word onset for either difficulty condition (p > .116). As typically observed (Friedman and Johnson, 2000), words that were later remembered elicited more positive-going PD-0332991 cell line waveforms over frontal scalp sites than words that were later forgotten (Fig. 5). Encoding-related activity elicited by words was quantified by measuring mean amplitudes in the 700–1200 and 1200–1900 msec intervals. These intervals were similar to those used to quantify post-stimulus subsequent

memory effects in previous investigations (e.g., Galli et al., 2011; Otten et al., 2006, 2010) and captured the effects in the group averaged waveforms for all relevant conditions. The ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between subsequent memory and scalp location in both latency intervals [respectively F(1, 27) = 7.04

and 9.13, p = .013 and .005]. Subsequent memory effects were largest over anterior scalp sites, but significant at both anterior locations [F(1, 27) = 16.83 and 18.91 for the two intervals, both p < .001] and posterior locations [F(1, 27) = 10.49 and 8.13, respectively, OSBPL9 p = .003 and .008]. No interactions involving modality or difficulty emerged (p > .117). The findings indicate that encoding-related activity before an event is sensitive to the degree to which processing resources are available. Electrical brain activity elicited by a cue presented just before word onset predicted later recall of the word, but only in a low demand situation when a concurrent task was easy to perform. Participants were asked to memorize short lists of words while making perceptual discriminations on cues that preceded the words. Discrimination difficulty was manipulated across lists by making the cues more or less similar to one another. The performance data show that cue discriminations were indeed faster and more accurate in the easy condition. The lower demands in this condition may have left sufficient opportunity to also engage brain activity that affects the encoding of the upcoming word. Accordingly, activity before word onset predicted later memory of the word.

The authors would like to apologize for any inconvenience caused

The authors would like to apologize for any inconvenience caused. Characterization of Xk(−/−) and Kel(−/−)/Xk(−/−) mice. The construct map of the targeted disruption of mouse Xk(−/−) is shown in the Figure S1. The targeting strategy of Xk was to replace a wild type 806-bp segment that includes partial 5′ end of exon 3 and its flanking intron 2 with a neomycin resistant gene cassette (1.85 kb). The neomycin resistant gene cassette contains an EcoRV site that wild type

Xk does not have resulting in different EcoRV restriction map in a Southern Blot analysis (Fig. S2). The wild type Xk yields check details two bands, 5.6 and 2.2 kb in size and the disrupted Xk gene yields 5.6 and 2.2 kb bands. The 2.2 kb-band is common in both genes, which could be used as an internal control for the southern blot analysis. The probe used for the Southern blot analysis was prepared from the fragment that includes only the middle EcoRV site shown in Fig. 1 as a filled oval circle. The description for Kel(−/−) gene and its Southern blot analysis was reported previously (1); Kel-yields 15 kb band and Kel + band yields 8 kb band upon digestion of genomic DNA with EcoRV in the Southern blot analysis. The mouse Xk has 80% amino acid similarity with human XK and is organized Selleck INK-128 in 3 exons as the human counterpart. The mouse Xk(−/−) gene and the wild type Xk gene are shown in the supplemental

figure S3. To produce Kel(−/−) or Xk(−/−) mice to have homogeneous C57BL/6 background, female Kel(−/−) or male Xk(−/y) mice were mated to C57BL/6 mice (Charles River Laboratories) http://www.selleck.co.jp/products/Adrucil(Fluorouracil).html and backcrossed for 10 generations by breeding heterozygous or hemizygous offspring with C57BL/6 mates. To generate double-knockout [Kel(−/−)/Xk(−/−)] mice, male Kel(−/−) mice with C57BL/6 background and female Xk(−/−) mice with C57BL/6 background were used in the initial mating. The phenotypes of the red cell

ghosts of the three knockout mouse lines with Xk(−/−), Kel(−/−) or Kel(−/−)/Xk(−/−) were analyzed by Western blot and compared with the results of the wild type mouse to confirm the absence of XK, Kell or both in the red blood cells of Xk(−/−), Kel(−/−) or Kel(−/−)/Xk(−/−) double knockout mice, respectively. The results are shown in the supplemental figure S4. As expected XK (lane 3 of left panel), Kell (lane 2 of right panel) or both XK and Kell (lanes 4 of both panels) are absent in the red blood cells of Xk(−/−), Kel(−/−) or Kel(−/−)/Xk(−/−) double knockout mice, respectively. Similar to human Kell null red blood cells and McLeod red blood cells, mouse Kel(−/−) red blood cells have markedly reduced level of XK protein (lane 2 of left panel probed with anti-XK) and Xk(−/−) red blood cells have markedly reduced level of Kell protein (lane 3 of right panel probed with anti-Kell), respectively. References 1.) X. Zhu, A. Rivera, M.S. Golub, et al.

Additionally, these lower-level species are commonly associated w

Additionally, these lower-level species are commonly associated with high rates of population turnover and an unstable abundance [6]. The SBSTTA stressed the importance of MTI as a biodiversity indicator, claiming that MTI is, “considered a particularly effective indicator to assess sustainability of fishing and the integrity of marine ecosystems” [6]. For this

reason, researchers affiliated with the CBD agree that policy decisions relating to the management of a species must include consideration of trophically-linked species [6]. Since the 2006 agreement by the Conference of the Parties to the CBD, several states obligated to the agreement have undergone national reviews of the application of MTI. These analyses discuss the possible incorporation of

MTI into national fisheries management decisions. Much of the current research Selleck GKT137831 has been centered in the European Union (EU), likely because the EU was among the earliest groups to adopt CBD indicators for biodiversity monitoring. In the summer of 2004, the EU Environment Council adopted 15 of the biodiversity indicators highlighted by the CBD, including MTI [17]. By 2005, the indicators were also adopted by the Pan-European Biological and Landscape Diversity Strategy Quizartinib [17]. The incorporation of MTI as a measure of diversity into two landmark Urease agreements in the European sphere represents a strong push toward sustainability on the European continent. In response to the continental push for MTI to be incorporated into fishery management decisions, pilot studies were performed by the British and Dutch in 2008 and 2007, respectively. In the United Kingdom (UK), significant effort has gone into the research and implementation of MTI into national policy decisions. The UK Biodiversity Partnership Standing Committee agreed upon 18 promising biodiversity indicators requiring additional follow-up to implement. Among the proposed indicators was MTI. In a 2008 report on the feasibility and accuracy of using MTI to estimate

ecosystem health in UK territorial waters, scientists concluded that the data needed to compute MTI was not available at a small-scale necessary to accurately predict MTI for the UK. Ultimately, the UK Committee recommended against the incorporation of an MTI indicator into management regimes, citing that, “data may not be representative of all trophic levels in UK waters, especially regionally” [18]. Similarly, a 2007 Dutch report was commissioned to examine the possibility of using MTI as an indicator for ecosystem health in the Netherlands territorial seas. The report identified MTI as offering, “the possibility to encapsulate data on fisheries landings in one figure, making changes in fisheries behavior visible in one glance” [17].

, 1998, Sagiv and Bentin, 2001 and Taylor et al , 2001c) Object-

, 1998, Sagiv and Bentin, 2001 and Taylor et al., 2001c). Object-based attentional effects (larger P1 for attended as compared to unattended faces) are also reported for faces (e.g.,

Gazzaley et al., 2008). Lexical decision tasks (requiring a word vs. non-word decision) allow the investigation of sensory-, syntactic- and semantic categorization processes. With respect to the P1 component, several studies have reported increased amplitudes with increasing orthographic neighborhood size (N), increasing word length, but decreasing word frequency, and decreasing orthographic typicality (e.g., Hauk and Pulvermüller, 2004, Hauk et al., 2006a, Hauk et al., 2006b and Segalowitz and Zheng, 2009; for a review, cf. Dien, 2009). According to Coltheart et al. (1977), N is a variable reflecting the orthographic relatedness of a letter string http://www.selleckchem.com/products/BIBF1120.html with words stored in memory. A large N indicates that many related words are stored in lexical

memory. This most likely elicits competition/inhibition which increases processing complexity during early categorization of a letter string. This seems to be indeed the case as e.g., the results from Hauk et al. (2009) show. A very similar interpretation applies for the effects of word length, because it is plausible to assume that long words increase processing complexity. In a study where the effects of word length were studied by controlling for the negative correlation with word frequency,

Hauk and Pulvermüller (2004) observed that long words produced a larger Z-VAD-FMK datasheet P1 than short words. An interesting aspect of the findings of Adenosine Hauk and Pulvermüller (2004) is that the latency of the P1-word length effect was shorter than that for word frequency. This finding suggests that word length affects early graphemic search/categorization processes that precede those related to accessing the lexicon. Thus, it appears that processing complexity affects the amplitude of the P1. If early categorization is difficult because processing complexity is high (for a large N and long words a large number of similar memory entries or features must be processed), the P1 tends to be large. A similar interpretation holds true for infrequent words and low orthographic typicality. Another interesting finding is that the P1 for words and pseudowords usually is of similar magnitude (e.g. Hauk et al., 2006a and Khateb et al., 2002). This is not surprising, if we consider the fact that pseudowords are constructed to exhibit a similar orthographic ‘surface characteristic’ as real words and that the P1 reflects early categorization (related to graphemic–phonetic features) that precedes access to lexical memory. Target-search paradigms clearly show that the P1 to the target stimulus is larger than the P1 to non-target stimuli (cf. the data reviewed by Taylor, 2002).